Music and Dance: The comparison between non-dancer's aesthetic experience and their Bodily sensations

  • Maja Vukadinović Novi Sad Business SchoolEducation Institution for Applied StudiesNovi Sad
Keywords: aesthetic experience, bodily sensations, listening to music, watching dance

Abstract


This paper aims at investigating and comparing the observers’ aesthetic experience and their bodily sensations in two different settings: when only listening to music and when watching dance choreographed to that particular music. The study included 209 students, non-dancers and non-musicians, aged between 17 and 27 (M = 19.39, SD = 1.37, 70.3% women) from Novi Sad Business School. The stimuli consisted of six audio-visual recordings of original dance performances including three contemporary and three hip hop pieces. The stimuli were presented in two different settings: in the first one, the participants only listened to the audio recordings of the music and in the second one they watched the audio-visual recordings of the choreographies made for that particular music. Each piece was assessed on two scales, one measuring the structure of aesthetic experience on three dimensions (Dynamism, Affective Evaluation and Exceptionality) and the other measuring the structure of bodily sensations (Focus, Excitement and Embodied Anticipation). The results have shown that the setting (music only/dance to music) had a statistically significant effect on the students’ assessments of their aesthetic experience and bodily sensations. The participants’ assessments of the dimensions of Affective Evaluation, Exceptionality and Focus were significantly higher when they watched dance choreographies made to the music which they had previously only listened to. Based on these results, it has been concluded that the higher the participants’ assessment of the delicacy, elegance and their attention to dance movements is, the higher is their engagement with the piece.

References

Beardsley, M. (1982). The aesthetic point of view. Ithaca N.Y.: Cornell University Press.

Bernardi, L., Porta, C., Sleight, P. (2006). Cardiovascular, cerebrovascular, and respiratory changes induced by different types of music in musicians and non-musicians: the importance of silence. Heart, 92(4), 445-452. https://doi.org/10.1136/hrt.2005.064600

Boso, M., Politi, P., Barale, F., & Emanuele, E. (2006). Neurophysiology and neurobiology of the musical experience. Functional Neurology, 21(4), 187.

Bynoe, Y. (2006). Encyclopedia of rap and hip-hop culture. Greenwood Press.

Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert Dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8), 1243–1249.https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhi007

Carrol, N. & Moore, M. (2012). Moving in concert: dance and music. In P. Goldie & Schellekens E. (Eds.), The aesthetic mind: Philosophy and psychology (pp.1–7). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Chin, T. C., Coutinho, E., Scherer, K. R., & Rickard, N. S. (2018). MUSEBAQ: A modular tool for music research to assess musicianship, musical capacity, music preferences, and motivations for music use. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 35(3), 376-399. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2018.35.3.376

Christensen, J. F. & Calvo-Merino, B. (2013). Dance as a subject for empirical aesthetics. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(1), 76–88. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031827

Christensen, J. F., Pollick, F. E., Lambrechts, A., & Gomila, A. (2016). Affective responses to dance. Acta Psychologica, 168, 91-105. ttps://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.03.008

Cervellin, G., & Lippi, G. (2011). From music-beat to heart-beat: a journey in the complex interactions between music, brain and heart. European Journal of Internal Medicine, 22(4), 371-374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejim.2011.02.019

Cross, E. S., Kirsch, L., Ticini, L.F. & Schütz-Bosbach, S. (2011). The impact of aesthetic evaluation and physical ability on dance perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5(102), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00102

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: the psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & Row.

Cvetičanin, P., & Popescu, M. (2011). The art of making classes in Serbia: Another particular case of the possible. Poetics, 39(6), 444-468. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.poetic.2011.09.006

Cvetičanin, P., Nedeljković, J., & Krstić, N. (2012). The cultural map of Serbia or the reconstruction of the field of cultural practices in Serbia. In P. Cvetičanin (Ed.), Social and cultural capital in Serbia (pp.71-96). Niš: Centre for Empirical Cultural Studies of South-East Europe.

Cupchik, G. C. (1974). An experimental investigation of perceptual and stylistic dimensions paintings suggested by art history. In D. E. Berlyne (Ed.), Studies in new experimental aesthetics (pp. 235–257). Washington (D.C.): Hemisphere

Publishing Corporation.

Damsholt, I. (2017). Identifying ‘choreomusical research’. In Music-Dance (pp. 17-34). Routledge.

Dodds, S. (Ed.). (2018). The Oxford handbook of dance and competition. USA: Oxford Handbooks.

Egermann, H., & Reuben, F. (2020). “Beauty is how you feel inside”: Aesthetic judgments are related to emotional responses to contemporary music. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 510029. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.510029

Fitch, W. T. (2016). Dance, music, meter and groove: a forgotten partnership. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 10, 64. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00064

Foster, S. (2008). Movement’s contagion: The kinesthetic impact of performance. In T. C. Davis (Ed.), The Cambridge companion to performance studies (pp. 46–59). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Foster, S. (2011). Choreographing empathy: Kinesthesia in performance. London and New York: Routledge.

Foster Vander Elst, O., Vuust, P., & Kringelbach, M.L. (2021). Sweet anticipation and positive emotions in music, groove, and dance. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 39, 79-84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.016

Glass, R. (2005). Observer response to contemporary dance. In Grove, R., Stevens, C. & McKechnie, S. (Eds.), Thinking in four dimensions: Creativity and cognition in contemporary dance (pp. 107–121). Carlton: Melbourne University Press.

Greenberg, D. M., Wride, S. J., Snowden, D. A., Spathis, D., Potter, J., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2022). Universals and variations in musical preferences: A study of preferential reactions to Western music in 53 countries. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 122(2), 286–309. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000397

Hagendoorn, I. (2011). Dance, choreography and the brain. In D. Melcher & F. Bacci (Eds.), Art and the senses (pp. 499-514). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Hanna, J. L. (1982). Is dance music? Resemblances and relationships. The World of Music, 24(1), 57–71. http://www.jstor.org/stable/43562654

Hoffmann, F. W. (2005). Rhythm and blues, rap, and hip-hop. Infobase Publishing.

Howlin, C., Vicary, S., & Orgs, G. (2020). Audiovisual aesthetics of sound and movement in contemporary dance. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 38(2), 191–211. https://doi.org/10.1177/0276237418818633

Janata, P. & Grafton, S. T. (2003). Swinging in the brain: shared neural substrates for behaviors related to sequencing and music. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 682-687. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1081

Janata, P., Tomic, S. T., & Haberman, J. M. (2012). Sensorimotor coupling in music and the psychology of the groove. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 54–75. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024208

Jola C. (2010). Research in choreography: merging dance and cognitive neuroscience. In B. Bläsing, Puttke, M., & Schack T. (Eds.), The Neurocognition of dance: mind, movement and motor skills (pp. 203-234). New York: Psychological Press.

Jola, C., Ehrenberg, S., & Reynolds, D. (2011). The experience of watching dance: Phenomenological-neuroscience duets. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 17-37. https://doi.org/1007/s11097-010-9191-x

Jola, C., & Grosbras, M. H. (2013). In the here and now: Enhanced motor corticospinal excitability in novices when watching live compared to video recorded dance. Cognitive Neuroscience, 4(2), 90-98. https://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2013.776035

Jola, C. & Christensen, J. F. (2015). Towards ecological validity in the research on cognitive and neural processes involved in dance appreciation. In M. Nadal, Huston, J. P., Agnati, L., Mora, F., and Cela-Conde, C. J. (Eds.). Art, aesthetics and the brain. Oxford University Press.

Jordan, S. (2011). Choreomusical conversations: Facing a double challenge. Dance Research Journal, 43(1), 43-64. https://doi.org/10.5406/danceresearchj.43.1.0043

Jovanovic, J. (2005). The power of recently revitalized Serbian rural folk music in urban settings. In A. J. Randall (Ed.), Music, power, and politics (pp.141-150). Routledge.

Kempe, M., & Heinen, T. (2022). Aesthetic perception of stage setups in dance. European Journal of Sport Sciences, 1(4), 29-35. https://doi.org/10.24018/ejsport.2022.1.4.30

Kirsch, L. P., Drommelschmidt, K. A., & Cross, E. S. (2013). The impact of sensorimotor experience on affective evaluation of dance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, Article 521. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00521

Koestler, A. (1970). The act of creation. London: Pan Books.

Krešić, I. (1997). Osnovni problemi umetničke igre. [The problems of dance as an form of art]. In S. Hrnjica, Panić, V., Radoš, K., & Krešić I. (Eds.), Psihologija (pp. 245-279). Beograd: Zavod za udžbenike i nastavna sredstva.

Krnjaić, Z., Videnović, M., Stepanović Ilić, I., & Krstić, K. (2020, October 15-18). Typical patterns of adolescents' musical preferences and activities during leisure [Conference presentation abstract]. Twenty-sixth international conference on Empirical Research in Psychology, Belgrade, Serbia. https://reff.f.bg.ac.rs/bitstream/handle/123456789/4346/KNJIGA-REZIMEA-2020.pdf?sequence=1&isAllowed=y

Kubovy, M. (1999). On the pleasures of the mind. In D. Kahneman, Diener E., & Schwartz N. (Eds.), Well-being: The foundation of hedonic psychology (pp.134–154). New York: Russell Sage.

Luck, G., Saarikallio, S., Burger, B., Thompson, M., & Toiviainen, P. (2014). Emotion-driven encoding of music preference and personality in dance. Musicae Scientiae, 18(3), 307-323.https://doi.org/10.1177/102986491453729

MacDonald, R., Kreutz, G., & Mitchell, L. (2012). Music, health and wellbeing. New York: Oxford University Press Inc.

Mandarić, S., & Jovančević, V. (2017, May 10-14). Relation between sex and aesthetic assessment of dancesport [Conference presentation]. Eight International Scientific Conference in Kinesiology, Opatija, Croatia. https://www.kif.unizg.hr/images/50005079/8_International_conference_kinesiology-2017.pdf

Macpherson, B. (2015). ‘Body musicality’: The visual, virtual, visceral voice. In K. Thomaidis & Macpherson B. (Eds.), Voice Studies. Critical approaches to process, performance and experience (pp. 175-187). Oxford: Routledge.

Madison, G. (2006). Experiencing groove induced by music: consistency and phenomenology. Music Perception, 24, 201–208. https://doi.org /10.1525/mp.2006.24.2.201

Marković, S. (2017). Domeni estetske preferencije. Od estetskog stimulusa do estetskog doživljaja. [Domains of the aesthetic preference. From the aesthetic stimulus to the aesthetic experience]. Beograd: Filozofski fakultet, Univerzitet u Beogradu.

Martin, J. (1939). Introduction to the dance. New York: Dance Horizons.

Mason, P. H. (2012). Music, dance and the total art work: choreomusicology in theory and practice. Research in Dance Education, 13(1), 5-24. https://doi.org/10.1080/14647893.2011.651116

Ognjenović, P. (2003). Psihološka teorija umetnosti. [The psychological theory of art]. Beograd: Gutenbergova galaksija.

Orgs, G., Hagura, N., & Haggard, P. (2013). Learning to like it: Aesthetic perception of bodies, movements and choreographic structure. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 22(2), 603–612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.03.010

Orgs, G., Caspersen, D., & Haggard, P. (2016). You move, I watch, it matters: Aesthetic communication in dance. In S. S. Obhi & Cross E.S. (Eds.), Shared representations: sensorimotor foundations of social life (pp. 627-654).Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Orgs, G., Calvo-Merino, B., & Cross, E. S. (2018). Knowing dance or knowing how to dance?: Sources of expertise in aesthetic appreciation of human movement. In B. Bläsing, Puttke, M., & Schack T. (Eds.), The neurocognition of dance (pp. 238–257). New York, NY: Routledge. http://dx.doi.org/10.4324/9781315726410-13

Patton, N. D. W. (1991). The influence of musical preference on the affective state, heart rate, and perceived exertion ratings of participants in aerobic dance/exercise classes. Texas Woman's University.

Petracovschi, S., Costas, C., & Voicu, S. (2011). Street dance: form of expressing identity in adolescents and youth. Timisoara Physical Education & Rehabilitation Journal, 3(6), 7-12.

Pflug, A. & Mandarić, S. (2012, February 10-11). Uloga pola ispitanika u estetskoj proceni društvenih plesova [The role of sex in the observers’ assessments of social dances] [Conference presentation abstract]. Eighteen international conference on Empirical Research in Psychology, Belgrade, Serbia. http://empirijskaistrazivanja.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/06/Knjiga-Rezimea-EIP-2012.pdf

Reasons, M. & Reynolds, D. (2010). Kinesthesia, empathy and related pleasures: An inquiry into audience experience of watching dance. Dance Research Journal, 42(2), 49-75. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0149767700001030

Reason, M., Jola, C., Kay, R., Reynolds, D., Kauppi, J.-P., Grobras, M.-H., Tohka, J., & Pollick, F. E. (2016). Spectators’ aesthetic experience of sound and movement in dance performance: A transdisciplinary investigation. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 10(1), 42–55. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040032

Reinhardt, U. (1999). Investigations into synchronization of heart rate and musical rhythm in a relaxation therapy in patients with cancer pain. Forschende Komplementarmedizin, 6(3), 135-141. https://doi.org/10.1159/000021235

Rentfrow, P. J., Goldberg, L. R., & Levitin, D. J. (2011). The structure of musical preferences: A five-factor model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(6), 1139–1157. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022406

Rentfrow, P. J., Goldberg, L. R., Stillwell, D. J., Kosinski, M., Gosling, S. D., & Levitin, D. J. (2012). The song remains the same: A replication and extension of the MUSIC model. Music perception, 30(2), 161-185. https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2012.30.2.161

Reynolds, D. & Reasons, M., (Eds.) (2012). Kinesthetic empathy in creative and cultural practice. USA: University of Chicago Press.

Reynolds, D., Reason, M., & Jola, C. (2014, April 24-26). Spectators’ aesthetic experiences of sound and movement in dance performance [Conference presentation abstract]. Second international conference on Cognitive Futures in the Humanities, Durham, United Kingdom. https://rke.abertay.ac.uk/ws/portalfiles/portal/15648127/Jola_Spectators_AestheticAbstract_2014.pdf

Reybrouck, M., Vuust, P., & Brattico, E. (2018). Brain connectivity networks and the aesthetic experience of music. Brain Sciences, 8(6), 107. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci8060107

Rose, D., Müllensiefen, D., Lovatt, P., & Orgs, G. (2020). The Goldsmiths dance sophistication index (Gold-DSI): A psychometric tool to assess individual differences in dance experience. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 16(4), 733–745. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000340

Schäfer, T., & Mehlhorn, C. (2017). Can personality traits predict musical style preferences? A meta-analysis. Personality and Individual Differences, 116, 265–273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.061

Schröder, J. H. (2017). Experimental relations between music and dance since the 1950s: Sketch of a typology 1. In P. Veroli & Vinay G. (Eds.), Music-Dance. Sound and motion in contemporary discourse (pp. 139-156). London: Routledge.

Silvia, P. J. (2012). Human emotions and aesthetic experience. In P. Shimamura & Palmer S. E. (Eds.), Aesthetic science: connecting minds, brain and experience (pp. 250-275). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Stevens, K. & McKechnie, S. (2005). Thinking in action: thought made visible in contemporary dance. Cognitive Processing, 6, (243–252). https://doi.org/ 10.1007/s10339-005-0014-x

Stevens, C. J., Schubert, E., Wang, S., Kroos, C., & Halovic, S. (2009). Moving with and without music: scaling and lapsing in time in the performance of contemporary dance. Music Perception, 26(5), 451-464.

Strukus, W. (2011). Mining the gap: Physically integrated performance and kinesthetic empathy. Journal of Dramatic Theory and Criticism, 25(2), 89-105.

Thaut, M. H, Trimarchi, P. D., & Parsons, L. M. (2014). Human brain basis of music rhythm perception: common and distinct neural substrates for meter, tempo and pattern. Brain Sciences, 4, 428-452. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci4020428

Thaut, M. H., McIntosh, G. C., & Hoemberg, V. (2015). Neurobiological foundations of neurologic music therapy: rhythmic entrainment and the motor system. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01185

Tormodsdatter Færøvik, H. U. (2017). Music and heart rate Physiological effects from listening to music in different tempos. Master Thesis. Bergen: Universitetet i Bergen det Psykologiske Fakultet.

Tsay, C. (2013). Sight over sound in the judgment of music performance. Proceedings of the National Academy of the Sciences USA 110, 14580–14585. https://doi.org/ 10.1073/pnas.1221454110

Verhaeghen, P. (2018). Once more, with feeling: The role of familiarity in the aesthetic response. The Psychological Record, 68, 379-384. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0312-1

Vukadinović, M. & Marković, S. (2012). Aesthetic experience of dance performances. Psihologija, 45(1), 23-41. https://doi.org/10.2298/PSI1201023V

Vukadinović, M. & Marković, S. (2022). Factor structure of audiences’ physical experience while watching dance. PsyCh Journal, 11(5), 660-672. https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.513

Vukadinović, M. (2013). An Audience’s Subjective Experience of the Freedom of Artistic Expression in Different Dance Forms from the Perspective of the Cultural Psychology of Creativity. Universitas Psychologica, 12 (3), 709-723.

Vukadinović, M. (2017a, March, 24-26). Contemporary dance: the influence of the choreographer’s style on the aesthetic experience of the choreographies [Conference presentation]. Twenty-third international conference on Empirical Research in Psychology, Belgrade, Serbia. http://empirijskaistrazivanja.org/wp-content/uploads/2017/10/Zbornik-EIP-2017.pdf

Vukadinović, M. (2017b, March, 24-26). The differences in the aesthetic experience of contemporary and hip-hop dance choreographies [Conference presentation]. Twenty-third international conference on Empirical Research in Psychology, Belgrade, Serbia. http://empirijskaistrazivanja.org/wp-content/uploads/2017/10/Zbornik-EIP-2017.pdf

Vukadinović, M. (2018, March 23-25). The relationship between observers’ somatic reactions and aesthetic experience when watching contemporary dance choreographies [Conference presentation abstract]. Twenty-fourth international conference on Empirical Research in Psychology, Belgrade, Serbia. http://empirijskaistrazivanja.org/wp-content/uploads/2020/01/Knjiga-Rezimea-EIP-18-c.pdf

Vukadinović, M. & Marković, S. (2017). The relationship between the dancers’ and the audience’s aesthetic experience. Psihologija, 50(4), 465-481. https://doi.org/10.2298/PSI160222009V

Vukadinović, M. (2019). Psihologija plesa i umetničke igre. [Psychology of Dance]. Sombor: Pedagoški fakultet, Novi Sad: Novosadski centar za istraživanje plesa i umetnost flamenka – La Sed Gitana.

Warrenburg, L. A., Reymore, L., & Shanahan, D. (2020). The communication of melancholy, grief, and fear in dance with and without music. Human Technology, 16(3), 283. https://doi.org/10.17011/ht/urn.202011256766

Woolhouse, M. H. & Lai, R. (2014). Traces across the body: influence of music-dance synchrony on the observation of dance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00965

Published
2024/01/04
Section
Scientific Articles