Cognitive ability and motor performances in the elderly

  • Stevan S. Jovanović Academy of Applied Studies, The College of Health Sciences, Belgrade, Serbia
  • Biljana N. Stojanović-Jovanović Academy of Applied Studies, The College of Health Sciences, Belgrade, Serbia
  • Aleksandra M. Pavlović University Clinical Center of Serbia, Neurology Clinic, Belgrade, Serbia
  • Radovan Lj. Milošević Military Medical Academy, Clinic for Urology, Belgrade, Serbia
  • Dragan M. Pavlović University of Belgrade, Faculty for Special Education and Rehabilitation, Belgrade, Serbia
Keywords: aging, cognition, exercises, aged, physical fitness

Abstract


Background/Aim. Aging entails a wide range of cognitive processes that are not independent of one another. It leads to changes in physical-motor characteristics and sometimes to disability. The aim of this study was to examine the association between multiple cognitive performances in elderly subjects and their physical-motor abilities. Method. The study included 98 elderly participants (60+) (16 males and 82 females). Cognitive abilities were assessed by the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA)/Serbian version, and physical measures were assessed by the Senior Fitness Test with its five subtests, supplemented by the Walking Speed Test. Results. Several MoCA items demonstrated relatively low variability, i.e., they proved to be too easy for most of the participants. The participants exhibited the lowest performance on the memory relating to other domains, followed by executive functions, visuospatial skills, attention, concentration, and working memory domains, with the highest performance on temporal and spatial orientation relating to other domains. Executive functions and language correlated most significantly with physical strength. Agility and dynamic balance, lower- and upper-body strength, and aerobic endurance correlated moderately and positively. Conclusion. This study underlines the positive correlation between physical fitness and cognitive level in the elderly and emphasizes the importance of physical fitness for cognitive functions, especially those of executive type in elderly subjects. Clinicians should consider the association between cognitive function and physical-motor performances when dealing with functioning improvement in the elderly. The importance of designing the most efficient exercise programs to achieve maximal somatic and cognitive effects is emphasized.

References

Zelinski E, Dalton S, Hindin S. Cognitive changes in healthy older adults. Generations 2011; 35(2): 13–20.

Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differ-ences in cognition. Psychol Rev 1996; 103(3): 403‒28.

Classon E, Fällman K, Wressle E, Marcusson J. Relations be-tween Concurrent Longitudinal Changes in Cognition, De-pressive Symptoms, Self-Rated Health and Everyday Function in Normally Aging Octogenarians. PLoS One 2016; 11(8): e0160742.

Cruz-Jentoft AJ, Baeyens JP, Bauer JM, Boirie Y, Cederholm T, Landi F, et al. European Working Group on Sarcopenia in Older People. Sarcopenia: European consensus on definition and diagnosis: Report of the European Working Group on Sarcopenia in Older People. Age Ageing 2010; 39(4): 412‒23.

Hiona A, Leeuwenburgh C. The role of mitochondrial DNA mu-tations in aging and sarcopenia: Implications for the mito-chondrial vicious cycle theory of aging. Exp Gerontol 2008; 43(1): 24–33.

Manini TM, Clark BC. Dynapenia and aging: an update. J Ger-ontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2012; 67(1): 28‒40.

Kido A, Tanaka N, Stein RB. Spinal excitation and inhibition decrease as humans age. Can J Physiol Pharmacol 2004; 82(4): 238‒48.

McNeil CJ, Doherty TJ, Stashuk DW, Rice CL. Motor unit number estimates in the tibialis anterior muscle of young, old, and very old men. Muscle Nerve 2005; 31(4): 461–7.

Rowe JB, Siebner H, Filipovic SR, Cordivari C, Gerschlager W, Rothwell J, et al. Aging is associated with contrasting changes in local and distant cortical connectivity in the human motor sys-tem. Neuroimage 2006; 32(2): 747–60.

Moon JH, Moon JH, Kim KM, Choi SH, Lim S, Park KS, et al. Sarcopenia as a Predictor of Future Cognitive Impairment in Older Adults. J Nutr Health Aging 2016; 20(5): 496‒502.

Blankevoort CG, Scherder EJA, Wieling MB, Hortobágyi T, Brouwer WH, Geuze RH, et al. Physical predictors of cognitive performance in healthy older adults: a cross-sectional analysis. PLoS One 2013; 8(7): e70799.

Montero-Odasso M, Verghese J, Beauchet O, Hausdorff JM. Gait and cognition: a complementary approach to understanding brain function and the risk of falling. J Am Geriatr Soc 2012; 60(11): 2127‒36.

Peel NM, Alapatt LJ, Jones LV, Hubbard RE. The Association Between Gait Speed and Cognitive Status in Community-Dwelling Older People: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2019; 74(6): 943‒8.

Rosso AL, Verghese J, Metti AL, Boudreau RM, Aizenstein HJ, Kritchevsky S, et al. Slowing gait and risk for cognitive impair-ment. Neurology 2017; 89(4): 336–42.

Lee S, Han J, Jin Y, Lee I, Hong H, Kang H. Poor physical fit-ness is independently associated with mild cognitive impair-ment in elderly Koreans. Biol Sport 2016; 33(1): 57–62.

Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, White-head V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool For Mild Cognitive Impair-ment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53(4): 695‒9.

Pavlović D, Pavlović A. Neuropsychological diagnostics. Bel-grade: Orion Art; 2013. (Serbian)

Kljajević V. Montreal Cognitive Assessment: The Serbian ver-sion. Current events in neurology, psychiatry, and border are-as. 2009; 17 (3‒4): 31‒9. (Serbian)

Freitas S, Simões MR, Marôco J, Alves L, Santana I. Construct Validity of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA). J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2012; 18(2): 242‒50.

Rikli RE, Jones CJ. Functional Fitness Normative Scores for Community-Residing Older Adults, Ages 60–94. J Aging Phys Act 1999; 7(2): 162–81.

Middleton A, Fritz SL, Lusardi M. Walking speed: the func-tional vital sign. J Aging Phys Act 2015; 23(2): 314–22.

Proskovec AL, Heinrichs-Graham E, Wilson TW. Aging modu-lates the oscillatory dynamics underlying successful working memory encoding and maintenance. Hum Brain Mapp 2016; 37(6): 2348–61.

Park DC, Festini SB. Theories of Memory and Aging: A Look at the Past and a Glimpse of the Future. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2017; 72(1): 82‒90.

Spaan PEJ. Episodic and semantic memory functioning in very old age: Explanations from executive functioning and pro-cessing speed theories. Cogent Psychol 2015; 2(1): doi: 1109782. https://doi.org/10.1080/23311908.2015.1109782.

Olaya B, Bobak M, Haro JM, Demakakos P. Trajectories of Ver-bal Episodic Memory in Middle-Aged and Older Adults: Evi-dence from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. J Am Geriatr Soc 2017; 65(6): 1274–81.

Nyberg L, Maitland SB, Rönnlund M, Bäckman L, Dixon RA, Wahlin Å, et al. Selective adult age differences in an age-invariant multifactor model of declarative memory. Psychol Aging 2003; 18(1): 149–60.

Rönnlund M, Nyberg L, Bäckman L, Nilsson LG. Stability, Growth, and Decline in Adult Life Span Development of De-clarative Memory: Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Data From a Population-Based Study. Psychol Aging 2005; 20(1): 3–18.

Ward EV, Berry CJ, Shanks DR. An effect of age on implicit memory that is not due to explicit contamination: Implica-tions for single and multiple-systems theories. Psychol Aging 2013; 28(2): 429–42.

Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12(3): 189‒98.

Cecato JF, Martinelli JE, Izbicki R, Yassuda MS, Aprahamian I. A subtest analysis of the Montreal cognitive assessment (MoCA): which subtests can best discriminate between healthy controls, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease? Int Psy-chogeriatr 2016; 28(5): 825–32.

Pavlović D, Pavlović A. Higher cortical functions. Neurology and neuropsychology of behavior. Belgrade: Orion Art; 2016. (Serbian)

Fjell AM, Sneve MH, Grydeland H, Storsve AB, Walhovd KB.The Disconnected Brain and Executive Function Decline in Aging. Cereb Cortex 2016; 27(3): 2303–17.

Lustig C, Jantz T. Questions of age differences in interference control: When and how, not if? Brain Res 2015; 1612: 59–69.

Ritchie SJ, Tucker-Drob EM, Cox SR, Corley J, Dykiert D, Red-mond P, et al. Predictors of ageing-related decline across multi-ple cognitive functions. Intelligence 2016; 59: 115–26.

Gouveia ÉR, Gouveia BR, Ihle A, Kliegel M, Marques A, Freitas DL. Balance and mobility relationships in older adults: A rep-resentative population-based cross-sectional study in Madeira, Portugal. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2019; 80: 65–9.

Eggermont LH, Gavett BE, Volkers KM, Blankevoort CG, Scherder EJ, Jefferson AL, et al. Lower-Extremity Function in Cogni-tively Healthy Aging, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alz-heimer’s Disease. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 2010; 91(4): 584–8.

Donoghue OA, Horgan NF, Savva GM, Cronin H, O’Regan C, Kenny RA. Association Between Timed Up-and-Go and Memory, Executive Function, and Processing Speed. Am Ger-iatr Soc 2012; 60(9): 1681–6.

Ibrahim A, Singh DKA, Shahar S. ‘Timed Up and Go’ test: Age, gender and cognitive impairment stratified normative values of older adults. PLoS One 2017; 12(10): e0185641.

Sheridan PL, Hausdorff JM. The role of higher-level cognitive function in gait: executive dysfunction contributes to fall risk in Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2007; 24(2): 125–37.

Falck RS, Wilcox S, Best JR, Chandler JL, Liu-Ambrose T. The Association Between Physical Performance and Executive Function in a Sample of Rural Older Adults from South Caro-lina, USA. Exp Aging Res 2017; 43(2): 192–205.

McGough EL, Kelly VE, Weaver KE, Logsdon RG, McCurry SM, Pike KC, et al. Limbic and Basal Ganglia Neuroanatomical Correlates of Gait and Executive Function. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2018; 97(4): 229–35.

Maguire EA, Intraub H, Mullally SL. Scenes, Spaces, and Memory Traces: What Does the Hippocampus Do? Neurosci-entist 2016; 22(5): 432‒9.

Corballis MC. Space, time, and language. Cogn Process 2018; 19(Suppl 1): 89‒92.

Piai V, Anderson KL, Lin JJ, Dewar C, Parvizi J, Dronkers NF, et al. Direct brain recordings reveal hippocampal rhythm un-derpinnings of language processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2016; 113(40): 11366‒71.

Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW, Fischer JS. Neuropsycho-logical assessment. Oxford: Oxford University Press, USA; 2004.

Rottschy C, Langner R, Dogan I, Reetz K, Laird AR, Schulz JB, et al. Modelling neural correlates of working memory: A coordi-nate-based meta-analysis. Neuroimage 2012; 60(1): 830–46.

Rhodes RE, Katz B. Working memory plasticity and aging. Psy-chol Aging 2017; 32(1): 51–9.

Sih R, Morley JE, Kaiser FE, Perry HM, Patrick P, Ross C. Tes-tosterone Replacement in Older Hypogonadal Men: A 12-Month Randomized Controlled Trial. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1997; 82(6): 1661‒7.

Janowsky JS, Oviatt SK, Orwoll ES. Testosterone influences spa-tial cognition in older men. Behav Neurosci 1994; 108(2): 325–32.

Narazaki K, Matsuo E, Honda T, Nofuji Y, Yonemoto K, Kumagai S. Physical fitness measures as potential markers of low cogni-tive function in Japanese community-dwelling older adults without apparent cognitive problems. J Sports Sci Med 2014; 13(3): 590–6.

Hörder H, Johansson L, Guo X, Grimby G, Kern S, Östling S, et al. Midlife cardiovascular fitness and dementia. Neurology 2018; 90(15): e1298–305.

Spartano NL, Ngandu T. Fitness and dementia risk: Further ev-idence of the heart-brain connection. Neurology 2018; 90(15): 675–6.

Kim HJ, Lee SY, Lee HG, Cho YH, Ko EM. Effects of a single-session cognitive enhancement fitness program on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive func-tion in middle-aged women. J Sports Sci Med 2018; 17(1): 110–6.

Published
2022/07/13
Section
Original Paper