Lexical-semantic representation of body parts in Serbian child language

  • Darinka Anđelković University of Belgrade - Faculty of Philosophy, Serbia https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8238-4460
  • Maja Savić University of Belgrade - Faculty of Philology, Belgrade
  • Maša Popović University of Belgrade - Faculty of Philosophy, Serbia
  • Milena Jakić Šimšić Institute for Serbian Language, Serbian Academy of Sciences and Arts, Belgrade, Serbia
Keywords: body parts, lexical development, semantic development, meronymy, early school age, Serbian language

Abstract


In this study we explore how the conception and lexicon of human body in the native Serbian-speaking adult language is mirrored in the conception and lexicon of Serbian-speaking children. We explored how children of 5, 7, and 9 years used words and expressions to refer to the human body and its parts, and how these corresponded to the segmentation and lexicalization of the body in adult language. Participants were asked to name the body parts depicted in the drawings presenting the whole body (front, back) and face, with a red dot marking the specific body part. The results show that, with respect to most of face parts, arms, and legs, there is a gradual conceptual segmentation of the human body with age, reflected in a systematic decrease in the use of holons and a systematic increase in the use of merons in naming body parts. However, such hierarchical partonomic organization was not confirmed for other parts (eye, nose, ear, trunk, shoulders, neck, head), indicating a "shallow" organization at the mental level and revealing different strategies in the acquisition of lexicon. Children of all ages, especially 7- and 9-year-olds, seek alternative solutions for the body parts whose names are not in their vocabulary. Thus, they name adjacent body parts, internal organs, parts of the skeleton, or describe the surrounding areas. The results are discussed in light of previous findings from comparative developmental and cross-linguistic studies with respect to the universality and linguistic specificity of the recorded regularities of lexical development.

References

Andersen, E. S. (1978). Lexical universals of body-part terminology. In J. H. Greenberg (Ed.), Universals in Human Language (Vol. 3, pp. 335-368). Stanford, CA. Stanford University Press.

Ayres, A. J. (1961). The development of the body scheme in children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 15(3), 99-128.

Auclair, L. & Jambaqué. I. (2015). Lexical-semantic body knowledge in 5- to 11-year-old children: How spatial body representation influences body semantics. Child Neuropsychology, 21(4), 451-464. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2014.912623

Bayley, N. (1969). Bayley Scales of Infant Development. New York, NY: Psychological Corporation.

Brown, C. H. (1976). General principles of human anatomical partonomy and speculations on the growth of partonomic nomenclature. American Ethnologist, 3, 400–424.

Brownell, C. A., Nichols, S. R., Svetlova, M., Zerwas, S., & Ramani, G. (2010). The head bone's connected to the neck bone: when do toddlers represent their own body topography?. Child development, 81(3), 797–810. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01434.x

Burenhult, N. (2006). Body part terms in Jahai. Language Sciences, 28(2-3), 162-180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2005.11.002

Buxbaum, L. J., & Coslett, H. B. (2001). Specialised structural descriptions for human body parts: Evidence from autotopagnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 18(4), 289–306. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290126172

Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Camões-Costa, V., Erjavec, M. & Horne, P.J. (2011). Comprehension and production of body part labels in 2- to 3-old children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 552-571. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151010X523040

Cratty, B. (1970). Perceptual and motor development in infants and children. New York: Macmillan.

Croft, W., & Cruse, A. D. (2004). Cognitive Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511803864>

Crowe, S. J., & Prescott, T. J. (2003). Continuity and change in the development of category structure: Insights from the semantic fluency task. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(5), 467–479. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250344000091

Devylder., S., Bracks, C., Shimogori, M., & Siahann, P (2020). Carving the body at its joints: Does the way we speak about the body shape the way we think about it? Language and Cognition, 12, 577–613. doi:10.1017/langcog.2020.13

Dilparić, B. M. (2012). On meronymy. Zbornik radova Filozofskog fakulteta u Prištini, 42-1, 279-307. https://scindeks.ceon.rs/article.aspx?artid=0354-32931201279D

 

Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Third. Edition. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

 

Enfield, N. J., Majid, A. & van Staden, M. (2006). Parts of the body: Cross-linguistic categorisation, special issue of Language Sciences, 28(2-3), 137-359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2005.11.001

Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D. J., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2006). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories, Second Edition (CDIs) [Database record]. APA PsycTests. https://doi.org/10.1037/t11538-000

Gellert, E. (1962). Children's conceptions of the content and functions of the human body. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 65, 293-405.

Gellert E. (1975). Children's constructions of their self-images. Perceptual and motor skills, 40(1), 307–324.

Gesell, A. (1940). The first five years of life. New York: Harper & Row.

Gesell, A., & Armatruda, C.S. (1947). Developmental Diagnosis: Normal and Abnormal Child Development, Clinical Methods and Pediatric Applications. 2nd ed. New York: Paul B. Hoeber; 1947.

Harris, D. B. (1963). Children drawings as measures of intellectual maturity. New York: Harcourt, Brace, and World.

Huisman, J. L. A., van Hout, & Majid, A. (2021). Patterns of semantic variation differ across body parts: Evidence from Japonic languages. Cognitive Linguistics, 32(3), 455–486 https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2020-007

Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (2002). Young children’s naive thinking about the biological world (Essays in Developmental Psychology). NY: Psychology Press-Taylor and Francis Group. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00431.x

Jaakkola, R. O. & Slaughter, V. (2002). Children's body knowledge: Understanding ‘life’ as a biological goal. Developmental Psychology, 20(3), 325-342.

https://doi.org/10.1348/026151002320620352

Johnson, C. N., & Kendrick, K. (1984). Body partonomy: How children partition the human body. Developmental Psychology, 20(5), 967–974.

https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.20.5.967

Jordan, F., Dunn, M. & Majid A. (2009). Body Part naming booklet. Developed for the EoSS project. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.

Liston, J. L. (1972). The Semantic Structure of Body-Part Terms in Serbo-Croatian: I. The Part-Whole Hierarchy. Anthropological Linguistics, 14, 8, 323-338.

Lucariello, J., Kyratzis, A., & Nelson, K. (1992). Taxonomic Knowledge: What Kind and When? Child Development, 63(4), 978–998. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131248

MacWhinney, K., Cermak, S. A., & Fisher, A. (1987). Body part identification in 1- to 4-year-old children. The American journal of occupational therapy : official publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association, 41(7), 454–459. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.41.7.454

Majid, A. (2010). Words for parts of the body. In Malt, B. C. & Wolff, P (Eds.): Words and the Mind: How Words Capture Human Experience. Oxford University Press. New York. Oxford. ISBN 978-0-19-531112-9.

Majid, A., Jordan, F. M., & Dunn, M. (2010). Evolution of Semantic Systems: Procedures Manual. Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen.

Majid, A. & Enfield, N. J. (2017). Body. In H. Burkhardt, J. Seibt, G. Imaguire, S. Gerogiorgakis (Eds.): Handbook of Mereology. Philosophia Verlag GmbH München. Germany.

Markman, E. M. (1981). Two different principles of conceptual organization. In M. Lamb & A. Brown (Eds.): Advances in developmental psychology (vol. I). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203728185

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.198.4312.75

Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54(3), 702–709. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130058

Müller, U., & Liebermann, D. (2004). Facing the body: toward a developmental theory of body knowledge. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69(2), 103–113. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0037-976X.2004.00293.x

Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: The Emergence of the Mediated Mind. Cambridge University Press, UK. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139174619>

Nelson, K. & Nelson, A. J. (1990). Category production in response to script and category cues by kindergarten and second grade children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 11(4), 431-446. https://doi.org/10.1016/0193-3973(90)90019-G

Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. (M. Cook, Trans.). W W Norton & Co. https://doi.org/10.1037/11494-000

Reggin, L. D., Muraki, E. J., & Pexman, P. M. (2021). Development of Abstract Word Knowledge. Frontiers in psychology, 12, 686478. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.686478

Rochat, P. (2010). The innate sense of the body develops to become a public affair by 2–3 years. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 738–745.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.021

Sell, M. A. (1992). The development of children's knowledge structures: Events, slots, and taxonomies. Journal of Child Language, 19(3), 659–676. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900011612

Sirigu, A., Grafman, J., Bressler, K., & Sunderland, T. (1991). Multiple representations contribute to body knowledge processing. Evidence from a case of autotopagnosia. Brain : a journal of neurology, 114 ( Pt 1B), 629–642. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/114.1.629

Slaughter, V., Heron, M., & Sim, S. (2002). Development of preferences for the human body shape in infancy. Cognition, 85(3), B71–B81. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00111-7

Slaughter, V., Heron, M., Tilse, E., Jenkins, L., Müller, U. & Liebermann, D. (2004). Origins and early development of human body knowledge: I. Levels of human body knowledge in development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 69(2), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0037-976X.2004.00287.x

Tincoff, R., & Jusczyk, P. W. (2012). Six-Month-Olds Comprehend Words That Refer to Parts of the Body. Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies, 17(4), 432–444. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00084.x

Terman, L. M., & Merrill, M. A. (1973). Stanford Binet Intelligence Scale: Manual for the Third Revision: 1972 Norms Edition, Houghton Mifflin, Boston.

Terril, A. (2006). Body part terms in Lukavele, a Papuan language of the Solomon Islands. Language Sciences, 28(2-3), 304-322. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2005.11.008

Waugh, W., & Brownell, C. (2015). Development of Body Part Vocabulary in Toddlers in Relation to Self-Understanding. Early child development and care, 185(7), 1166–1179. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2014.983915

Witt, A., Cermak, S., & Coster, W. (1990). Body part identification in 1- to 2-year-old children. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 44(2), 147–153. https://doi.org/10.5014/ajot.44.2.147

Wolff, P. M. & Malt, B. C. (2010). The language-thought interface: An introduction. In B. C. Malt & P. M. Wolff (Eds.): Words and the Mind: How words capture human experience. Oxford University Press. New York. Oxford.

Yu, Y., & Nelson, K. (1993). Slot-filler and conventional category organisation in young Korean children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 16(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1177/016502549301600101

Published
2023/06/30
Section
Scientific Articles