Fonološka svesnost, verbalna radna memorija i brzo automatsko imenovanje kao pokazatelji razvijenosti rečnika kod dece predškolskog uzrasta

  • Nevena Ječmenica Univerzitet u Beogradu – Fakultet za specijalnu edukaciju i rehabilitaciju, Beograd, Srbija
  • Slavica Golubović Univerzitet u Beogradu – Fakultet za specijalnu edukaciju i rehabilitaciju, Beograd, Srbija
Ključne reči: fonološka svesnost, ekspresivni rečnik, receptivni rečnik

Sažetak


Uvod: Prema modelu leksičkog restrukturiranja, razvoj rečnika pokreće razvoj fonoloških reprezentacija. Stoga se može predvideti da će deca sa razvijenim rečnikom posedovati vrlo specifične fonološke reprezentacije reči. Cilj: Cilj istraživanja je da se utvrdi značaj fonološke svesnosti, verbalne radne memorije i brzog automatskog imenovanja kao pokazatelja razvijenosti ekspresivnog i receptivnog rečnika kod dece predškolskog uzrasta. Metode: Istraživanjem je obuhvaćeno 86 dece tipičnog razvoja. Za procenu fonološke svesnosti, verbalne radne memorije, brzog automatskog imenovanja, ekspresivnog i receptivnog rečnika korišćeni su suptestovi koji predstavljaju deo Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals – Fourth Edition baterije. Rezultati: Rezultati istraživanja pokazuju da model koji objedinjuje uzrast, fonološku svesnost, verbalnu radnu memoriju i brzo automatsko imenovanje predviđa 51% varijanse postignuća na zadacima procene ekspresivnog rečnika (F = 7.73, df1 = 2, df2 = 75, p < .001) i 38% varijanse postignuća na zadacima procene receptivnog rečnika (F = 4.65, df1 = 2, df2 = 75, p < .001). U konačnom modelu kao statistički značajni pokazatelji ekspresivnog rečnika ističu se samo zadaci identifikacije i supstitucije fonema, kao i brzo automatsko imenovanje (p < .05). Sa druge strane, kao statistički značajni pokazatelji receptivnog rečnika ističu se analiza i sinteza fonema, verbalna radna memorija i brzo automatsko imenovanje (p < .05). Zaključak: Dobijeni rezultati pokazali su da uzrast, fonološka svesnost, verbalna radna memorija i brzo automatsko imenovanje imaju značajan doprinos u razvoju rečnika kod dece pre formalne obuke čitanja.

Reference

Ainsworth, S., Welbourne, S., & Hesketh, A. (2016). Lexical restructuring in preliterate children: Evidence from novel measures of phonological representation. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37, 997-1023. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716415000338

Altvater-Mackensen, N., & Fikkert, P. (2010). The acquisition of the stop-fricative contrast in perception and production. Lingua, 120, 1898-1909. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.02.010

Anglin, J. M. (1989). Vocabulary growth and the knowing-learning distinction. Reading Canada, 7, 142-146.

Anthony, J. L., Williams, J. M., McDonald, R., Corbitt-Shindler, D., Carlson, C. D., & Francis, D. J. (2006). Phonological processing and emergent literacy in Spanish-speaking preschool children. Annals of Dyslexia, 56(2), Article 239. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-006-0011-5

Aslin, R. N., & Smith, L. B. (1988). Perceptual development. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 435-473. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.39.020188.002251

Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11), 417-423. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2

Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. Oxford University Press.

Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105(1), 158-173.

Bailey, T. M., & Plunkett, K. (2002). Phonological specificity in early words. Cognitive Development, 17(2), 1265-1282. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00116-8

Ballem, K. D., & Plunkett, K. (2005). Phonological specificity in children at 1;2. Journal of Child Language, 32(1), 159-173. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000904006567

Baumann, J. F., & Kame’enui, E. J. (1991). Research on vocabulary instruction: Ode to Voltaire. In J. Flood, J. J. D. Lapp, & J. R. Squire (Eds.), Handbook of research on teaching the English language arts (1st ed., pp. 604-632). Macmillan.

Beatie, R. L., & Manis, F. R. (2014). The relationship between prosodic perception, phonological awareness and vocabulary in emergent literacy. Journal of Research in Reading, 37(2), 119-137. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2011.01507.x

Bowers, P. G. (1995). Tracing symbol naming speed’s unique contributions to reading disabilities over time. Reading and Writing, 7(2), 189-216. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01027185

Bowey, J. A. (1996). On the association between phonological memory and receptive vocabulary in five-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63(1), 44-78. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.0042

Bowey, J. A., McGuigan, M., & Ruschena, A. (2005). On the association between serial naming speed for letters and digits and word-reading skill: Towards a developmental account. Journal of Research in Reading, 28(4), 400-422. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2005.00278.x

Brown, G. D., & Hulme, C. (1996). Nonword repetition, STM, and word age-of-acquisition: A computational model. In S. Gathercole (Ed.), Models of short-term memory (pp. 129-148). Psychology Press.

Bryant, P., MacLean, M., & Bradley, L. (1990). Rhyme, language, and children’s reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11(3), 237-252. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400008870

Burgess, S. R., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Bidirectional relations of phonological sensitivity and prereading abilities: Evidence from a preschool sample. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70(2), 117-141. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1998.2450

Carroll, J. M., Snowling, M. J., Stevenson, J., & Hulme, C. (2003). The development of phonological awareness in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 39(5), 913-923. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012–1649.39.5.913

Casserly, E. D., & Pisoni, D. B. (2010). Speech perception and production. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(5), 629-647. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.63

Claessen, M., Heath, S., Fletcher, J., Hogben, J., & Leitão, S. (2009). Quality of phonological representations: A window into the lexicon? International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 44(2), 121-144. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820801966317

Cooper, D. H., Roth, F. P., Speece, D. L., & Schatschneider, C. (2002). The contribution of oral language skills to the development of phonological awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23(3), 399-416. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716402003053

Diehl, R. H., Lotto, A. J., & Holt, L. L. (2004). Speech perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 149-179. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142028

Elbro, C., Borstrøm, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1998). Predicting dyslexia from kindergarten: The importance of distinctness of phonological representations of lexical items. Reading Research Quarterly, 33(1), 36-60. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.33.1.3

Evans, M. A., Shaw, D., & Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 65-75. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0087330

Fadiga, L., Craighero, L., Buccino, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Speech listening specifically modulates the excitability of tongue muscles: A TMS study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 15(2), 399-402. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01874.x

Felton, R. H., & Brown, I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2(1), 39-59. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00383373

Fowler, A. E. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phonological awareness. In S. Brady, & D. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 97-117). Erlbaum.

Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. A. (2009). Effects of onset density in preschool children: Implications for development of phonological awareness and phonological representation. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30(2), 339-361. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716409090146

Galantucci, B., Fowler, C. A., & Turvey, M. T. (2006). The motor theory of speech perception reviewed. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 361-377. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193857

Garlock, V. M., Walley, A. C., & Metsala, J. L. (2001). Age-of-acquisition, word frequency and neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition by children and adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 45(3), 468-492. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2784

Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28(2), 200-213. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90044-2

Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory & Language, 29, 336-360. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)90004-J

Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Phonological working memory: A critical building block for reading development and vocabulary acquisition? European Journal of Psychology of Education, 8(3), 259-272. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03174081

Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., & Baddeley, A. D. (1991). Differentiating phonological memory and awareness of rhyme: Reading and vocabulary development in children. British Journal of Psychology, 82(3), 387-406. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1991.tb02407.x

Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Emslie, H., & Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 28(5), 887-898. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.887

Gibbs, S. (2004). Phonological awareness: An investigation into the developmental role of vocabulary and short-term memory. Educational Psychology, 24(1), 13-25. https://doi.org/10.1080/0144341032000146412

Goodrich, J. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2015). Lexical characteristics of words and phonological awareness skills of preschool children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(6), 1509-1531. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716414000526

Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(5), 393-402. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2113

Hulme, C., Thomson, N., Muir, C., & Lawrence, A. (1984). Speech rate and the development ofshort-term memory span. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38(2), 241-253. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(84)90124-3

Leseman, P. P. M., & de Jong, P. F. (1998). Home literacy: Opportunity, instruction, cooperation and social-emotional quality predicting early reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 33(3), 294-318. https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.33.3.3

Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(1), 1-38. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99001776

Lonigan, C. J., Anthony, J. L., Phillips, B. M., Purpura, D. J., Wilson, S. B., & McQueen, J. D. (2009). The nature of preschool phonological processing abilities and their relations to vocabulary, general cognitive abilities, and print knowledge. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2), 345-358. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013837

McBride-Chang, C., Wagner, R. K., & Change, L. (1997). Growth modeling of phonological awareness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(4), 621-630. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.4.621

McDowell, K. D., Lonigan, C. J., & Goldstein, H. (2007). Relations among socioeconomic status, age, and predictors of phonological awareness. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50(4), 1079-1092. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/075)

Metsala, J. L. (1999). Young children’s phonological awareness and nonword repetition as a function of vocabulary development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(1), 3-19. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.1.3

Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In J. L. Metsala, & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89-120). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Michas, I. C., & Henry, L. A. (1994). The link between phonological memory and vocabulary acquisition. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12(2), 147-163. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1994.tb00625.x

Penno, J. F., Wilkinson, I. A. G., & Moore, D. W. (2002). Vocabulary acquisition from teacher explanation and repeated listening to stories: Do they overcome the Matthew effect? Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(1), 23-33. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.94.1.23

Roth, F. P., & Spekman, N. J. (1986). Narrative discourse: Spontaneously generated stories of learning-disabled and normally achieving students. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 51(1), 8-23. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5101.08

Savage, R., Pillay, V., & Melidona, S. (2008). Rapid serial naming is a unique predictor of spelling in children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(3), 235-250. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219408315814

Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals (4th ed.). Psychological Corporation.

Sénéchal, M., Ouellette, G., & Rodney, D. (2006). The misunderstood giant: On the predictive role of early vocabulary to future reading. In D. K. Dickinson, & S. B. Neuman (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 173-184). Guilford Press.

Silvén, M., Niemi, P., & Voeten, M. J. M. (2002). Do maternal interaction and early language predict phonological awareness of 3- to 4-year old children? Cognitive Development, 17(1), 1133-1155. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00093-X

Snowling, M., Chiat, S., & Hulme, C. (1991). Words, nonwords, and phonological processes: Some comments on Gathercole, Willis, Emslie, and Baddeley. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12(3), 369-373. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400009279

Stadler, M. A., Watson, M., & Skahan, S. (2007). Rhyming and vocabulary: Effects of lexical restructuring. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 28(4), 197-205. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740107311823

Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 934-947. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.934

Swingley, D. (2009). Onsets and codas in 1.5-year-olds’ word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 60(2), 252-269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.11.003

Swingley, D., & Aslin, R. N. (2002). Lexical neighborhoods and the word-form representations of 14-month-olds. Psychological Science, 13(5), 480-484. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00485

Taylor, H. G., Lean, D., & Schwartz, S. (1989). Pseudoword repetition ability in learning-disabled children. Applied Pycholinguistics, 10(2), 203-219. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400008523

Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second-to fifth-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1(2), 161-185. https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr0102_4

Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101(2), 192-212. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.192

Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Laughon, P., Simmons, K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1993). Development of young readers’ phonological processing abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(1), 83-103.

Werker, J. F., & Curtin, S. (2005). PRIMIR: A developmental framework of infant speech processing. Language Learning and Development, 1(2), 197-234. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15473341lld0102_4

Werker, J. F., Fennell, C. T., Corcoran, K. M., & Stager, C. L. (2002). Infants’ ability to learn phonetically similar words: Effects of age and vocabulary size. Infancy, 3(1), 1-30. https://doi.org/10.1207/15250000252828226

Wise, J. C., Sevcik, R. A., Morris, R. D., Lovett, M. W., & Wolf, M. (2007). The relationship among receptive and expressive vocabulary, listening comprehension, pre-reading skills, word identification skills, and reading comprehension by children with reading disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 1093-1109. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/076)

Wolf, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly, 26(2), 123-141. https://doi.org/10.2307/747978

Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 387-407. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221940003300409

Wolf, M., Pfeil, C., Lotz, R., & Biddle, K. (1994). Towards a more universal understanding of the developmental dyslexias: The contribution of orthographic factors. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge I: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 137-171). Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-3492-9_5

Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 3-29.

Objavljeno
2021/12/14
Rubrika
Originalni naučni članak